2.2.3.1
Behaviourist accounts

Early behaviourist accounts (see Ellis, R, 1994: 81) acknowledged that first language learning did not involve conscious intention or understanding of language form. They assumed that children developed their first language through endless repetition of language they heard around them. Adults would then provide feedback and reinforcement to ensure that they produced language correctly. But research on first language acquisition showed that children don't just automatize a repertoire of learned phrases. They also manage to induce some kind of internal grammar on their own, and as any parent will know, they often don't pay attention to adults' feedback, whether explicit as in the first dialogue below or more implicit as in the second.

Child: Nobody don't like me
Mother: No, say 'nobody likes me'
Child

Nobody don't like me
[8 repetitions of this exchange]
Mother No, now listen carefully; say: 'nobody likes me'
Child Oh! Nobody don't likes me.
(McNeill, 1966: 69, cited in Gass and Selinker, 1994: 61)
Child My teacher holded the baby rabbits and we patted them.
Adult Did you say your teacher held the baby rabbits?
Child Yes.
Adult What did you say she did?
Child She holded the baby rabbits and we patted them.
Adult Did you say she held them tightly?
Child No, she holded them loosely.
(Cazden, 1972: 92 cited in Gass and Selinker, 1994: 61)

In fact, it seems that adults rarely correct children's grammar, even if they do correct socially inappropriate language use (eg 'Say please') and inappropriate vocabulary use (eg 'That's not a microphone - that's a necklace'). Children seem to build up their internal grammar through implicit induction, by subconsciously working out the patterns of the grammar on their own.

But is this an entirely plausible explanation, given the complexity of human grammars? In section 2.2.1, we sketched out the challenge facing second language learners. The challenge facing children is similar, if not greater given their less sophisticated cognitive powers. Just to remind yourself of that challenge, Reflective task 14 offers you an opportunity to try your hand at induction of some language patterns.

Reflective task 14

Exercise 1

  1. Look at the list of French nouns and their articles. They are not presented in any particular order, and it does not matter if you don't know what they mean.

    la station le potage la rage
    le rendement l'enseignement la correction
    la passion la cage le fondement
    le garage la liberté le visionnement
    l'action la plage l'appartement
    la coopération le passage l'éternité
    la nationalité l'irritation l'ornement


  2. Now complete the following blanks before these French-like nonsense words.

    __ trivernement __ éborage __ iroment
    __ tage __ ranté __ uration
    __ métoration __ lavorage __ vission

Exercise 2

  1. Now look at the following data in English:
    John is happy to please - he is happy to please his boss
    John is eager to please - he is eager to please his boss


  2. Based on the pattern, complete the following sentence:
    John is easy to please: he is _____________________

  3. What do you notice?

What conclusions do you draw from doing these exercises?

Click on Commentary for feedback.